Link: https://twitter.com/laura_tastic/status/1022890688525029376
Also, they picked up on this it at the incidental economist and gave a good summary of the key papers here.Do you use diff-in-diff? Then this thread is for you.— Laura Hatfield (@laura_tastic) July 27, 2018
You’re no dummy. You already know diverging trends in the pre-period can bias your results.
But I’m here to tell you about a TOTALLY DIFFERENT, SUPER SNEAKY kind of bias.
Friends, let’s talk regression to the mean. (1/N) pic.twitter.com/M2tEEsBiyH
You can find citations for the relevant papers below. I won't plagerize what both Laura and the folks at the Incidental Economist have already explained very well. But, at a risk of oversimplifying the big picture I'll try to summarize a bit. Matching in a few special cases can improve the precision of the estimate in a DID framework, and occasionally reduces bias. Remember, that matching on pre-period observables is not necessary for the validity of difference in difference models. There are cases when the treatment group is in fact determined by pre-period outcome levels. In these cases matching is necessary. At other times, if not careful, matching in DID introduces risks for regression to the mean…what Laura Hatfield describes as a ‘bounce back’ effect in the post period that can generate or inflate treatment effects when they do not really exist.
Both the previous discussion on DID in a GLM context and combining matching with DID indicate the risks involved in just plug and play causal inference and the challenges of bridging the gap between theory and application.
References:
Daw, J. R. and Hatfield, L. A. (2018), Matching and Regression to the Mean in Difference‐in‐Differences Analysis. Health Serv Res, 53: 4138-4156. doi:10.1111/1475-6773.12993
Daw, J. R. and Hatfield, L. A. (2018), Matching in Difference‐in‐Differences: between a Rock and a Hard Place. Health Serv Res, 53: 4111-4117. doi:10.1111/1475-6773.13017
No comments:
Post a Comment